A so-called conservation biologist at St. Lawrence
University in Canton, New York, named Susan Willson has five cats, two that
live outside and three indoors. One of the outdoor cats, named “the Gorilla,”
is an admitted “serial killer” cat, one that is “especially adept at killing
birds.” She tried but decided not to keep this cat indoors because it gets into
“a peeing war” with her three indoor cats, so she is willing to accept her cat
killing a lot of birds because she does not want to euthanize her cat. All
things considered, she would fall under what many conservation biologists call
a “cat lady.”
But she evidently feels at least some guilt over the
needless loss of wildlife at her hands because she bought a Birdsbesafe Collar
Cover that is claimed to reduce bird kills, and then conducted a study to at least
partly justify the collar cover as adequate mitigation for those wildlife kills.
Her study is reported in Birdsbesafe:
Can a novel cat collar reduce avian mortality by domestic cats (Felis catus)? S.K. Willson, I.A. Okunlola,
and J.A. Novak. Global Ecology and
Conservation 3(2015) 359-366.
Birdsbesafe’s product consists of a tube of brightly
colored cloth enclosing a break-away nylon collar and having a reflective edge
trim to make it easier to see by motorists at night. Collar covers are brightly
colored so that birds and some other prey species can more easily spot cats stalking
them. Reds and oranges are used extensively along with yellows; however, they noted
that reds, oranges, and yellows can match autumn leaf colors and thus might camouflage
cats somewhat and be responsible for the increase observed in autumn bird
predation. Many mammals, including nearly all nocturnal mammals, cannot see red
colors, rendering Birdsbesafe’s product of little value to them. Conversely,
reds and yellows possibly come across as camouflage to color-blind mammals
under certain conditions. Also, collar covers reflecting ultraviolet could provide
additional visual warning to some wildlife, but this was not addressed.
Willson et al. found that 71% of tested cats got used to collar
covers within 5 days. That means the collar covers will be problematic for 29%
of the kind of cats they tested. Since all tested cats were fully domesticated
human-owned animals, it is probable that feral cats would exhibit significantly
lower collar cover acceptance rates. In combination with the difficulty in
trapping and then re-trapping feral cats to install and replace lost break-away
collars, effective use rates are likely to be quite low and thus ineffective
mitigation. This point was not reported by authors or noted by Birdsbesafe on its
website.
Birdsbesafe collar covers are made of an unidentified
“fabric” and designed to enclose a nylon collar, both of which will absorb and
hold water and other liquids. If the product is never washed, such as with
feral cats, cat hoarders, and other low-quality cat owners, collar covers can
become stiff with embedded algae and dirt (and age). This could result in open
sores on the cats from abrasion and could snag on brush and fencing when running
from predators. This may not be an issue in a fenced-in suburban back yard, but
both issues could constitute cruelty when applied to feral cats whose collars cannot
be replaced due to trap-shyness. Willson et al. and Birdsbesafe do not address
this issue even though authors suggest that collar covers be used on feral
cats.
Willson et al. tested the successfulness of collar covers
only on serial killer cats like “the Gorilla.” They placed collar covers on 54
cats in the fall and 19 cats in the spring for twelve weeks each, and claimed that
collar covers reduced bird kills by a factor of 19 times in the spring and 3.4
times in the fall and decreased small mammal predation by about half in the
fall but had no effect in the spring. In comparison, belled collars were shown
by a second research group to reduce bird and mammal kills by 50%, although a
third study revealed that cats can compensate for the bells and develop
different hunting strategies. A fourth study showed that the CatAlert product,
which produces a beeping sound every seven seconds, resulted in cat kill
reductions that were not significantly different from bells. The CatBib is a
large piece of colored cloth attached to a collar that not only visually warns
birds and color-vision enabled mammals but also places a physical barrier
between cat paws and wildlife victims. A fifth study found that 81% of
CatBibbed cats stopped catching birds and 45% stopped catching mammals, but
noted that owner acceptance would likely be small because the CatBib is bulky
and appears awkward.
The serial killer cats that were employed in the
Bridsbesafe collar cover study habitually brought home killed but allegedly “intact”
prey. Obviously, this protocol does not account for kills that were simply left
in the field, stolen by other predators, or eaten. This could be a significant
flaw in the data because large numbers of uncounted kills could go unreported:
Assume that an uncollared cat over
a week’s time kills 10 birds, eats 2 of them, and brings 8 home. Assume that a
Birdsbesafe collar cover is then put on that same cat and it then kills only 2
birds but eats both of them over the next week. Willson et al.’s conclusions
would then be that collar covers reduced kills from 8 to 0, or by 100%, when
the real rate would be 80%.
Alternatively, assume that the
uncollared cat over a week again killed 10 birds, ate 2 of them, and brought 8
home, but a collar cover used over the following week resulted in only 4 bird
kills, 2 of which were eaten and the other 2 brought home. This would result in
a calculated 75% reduction in kills when the actual number would be only 60%.
Birdsbesafe and Willson et al. can play with the numbers
and come up with any success rate they want, but any way that I look at the
numbers, authors’ results are optimistic and may actually be gross
overestimates of success because they do not contain a correction factor to
account for kills not brought home. Has any research been done on quantifying
such a correction factor? I did a lot of literature reviewing on this subject a
few years ago and do not remember any such correction factor being published in
the scientific literature, but if a correction factor does exist, I am sure
that a scientist like Willson at an institute of higher learning like St.
Lawrence University would have applied it. If a correction factor does not
exist and if it is potentially important, then why do Willson’s and other
research groups not ascertain it? It would clearly be a significant gap in our
knowledge of cat kills and cat prey mitigation.
Willson et al.’s data collection occurred during two
seasons, spring and fall. There were no reasons cited for those seasons nor for
not sampling during other seasons. I suspect it had to do with summer college
vacations and winter cold, but authors should have addressed whether these two
seasons are representative of a full year or else should have pointed out that
their results may not be indicative of an annual period. Birdsbesafe’s 87%
reduction claim implies an annual basis, which I think is misleading.
Willson et al.’s tested cats brought home 19 species of
birds from 12 families and 11 species of mammals from 8 families, totaling 50
birds and 136 mammals. Unfortunately, authors did not include a raw-numbers table
of before-and-after kills, so we cannot scrutinize the actual data ourselves. The
only raw data provided by authors was a list of kill numbers by species, and
the only reduced data provided is a bar chart of average kill numbers per cat
per week. They then report their results in terms of reduction multiples,
stating that uncollared cats killed 19 times more birds in the spring and 3.4
times more in the fall. Similarly, uncollared mammal kills were twice as high
as collar-cover kills in the fall but were equal to collar-cover kills in the
spring. In other words, if I have done my math correctly in the absence of raw
data, collar covers reduced bird kills by 77-95% and mammal kills by 0-50%.
However, considering the uncounted-kills flaw, those numbers may be overestimates
by 10%, 20%, or even more.
Willson et al. reported that only one herpetofaunal
(herp) prey item was brought home, a spring peeper frog. They claimed that the
reason for this low number is that the study was done in northern New York,
implying that this locale has low herp populations. I am not sure if that is
true, but even if it is, the implication should have been clearly stated as a
fact and should have been referenced.
They quote a sixth report that free-ranging cats wearing
kitty-cams caught more herps than any other type of animal. Additional reports have
found that cat owners are more likely to bag and freeze certain animals than
others. Owners (1) readily collect small birds and intact (non-bloody) mammals
and lizards, (2) are less likely to handle shredded carcasses, (3) are unlikely
to even see, much less collect small herps half-eaten and left by the cat in
the lawn, and (4) snakes of any kind or size. Therefore, it is likely that Willson
et al.’s tested cats did bring home more herps than owners were willing to
collect and report, so authors were correct to eliminate herps from their
study.
The generally accepted and most often way in the
biological literature to convey before-and-after changes is to report percent
changes, whereas Willson et al. reported only reduction multiples. The latter
are indeed fine to impart, but only if percent changes are also reported. Authors’
peers cannot otherwise confirm the original findings, which is important if
herps or other prey items are left out of the equation.
Willson et al.’s data is based on kills that were
collected by cat owners and saved for seasonal pickup, thus relying on the cat
owner claims that they saved all the brought-home kills. It is known from other
studies that many prey items brought home are in varying states of intactness
and that owners will leave a particularly stinky mess on the lawn rather than
bag and freeze it. This was not mentioned by the authors even though it is a
known flaw of owner-based data collection and will underestimate predation
rates.
Willson et al. initially used collars that easily broke
away and were lost approximately 50% of the time. They replaced that model with
one that broke away less than 10% of the time. Obviously, different break-away
rates will produce different efficacy rates, but authors neither gave different
efficacy rates for the two collar models nor reported whether the two data sets
were determined separately or lumped together. This should have been clarified.
Regardless, authors did not name the brands/models of the tested collars, contrary
to standard methods. This failure means that their experiment cannot be
independently repeated by others.
Willson et al. did not address whether any of the tested
cats learned how to open a break-away collar or if the broken collars were
accidentally separated. Although not integral to their study, cats may find it
easier to learn how to disengage some collar models than others, and this might
be why one of their two models broke away so often. This is yet another reason
why authors should have listed the collar models tested.
Only 26% of Willson et al.’s participating cat owners
said they would buy and use cover collars in the future. Their reasons were
break-away losses, cat comfort, and owner dislike of the product. It is good to
note that Birdsbesafe developed a new break-away collar from stretchy nylon
fabric in order to lessen the break-away issue.
Willson et al. focus on bird kill reductions and discuss
mammal kill reductions but little. This is perplexing, since mammals are as
ecologically valuable as birds and cats evidently kill more mammals than birds.
It is obvious to conservation biologists that it is at least as important to
protect mammals as birds. True, Birdsbesafe collar covers are only claimed to
reduce bird predation, but the correct goal is to protect all wildlife from
free-roaming cats, so a birds-only mitigation device is woefully inadequate. It
begs the question of why authors expended resources on a product with such
limited value.
In summary, Willson et al.’s report is flawed in terms of
(1) its reason for being, which was to assuage guilt, (2) non-standard and misleading
methods of reporting kill reductions, (3) failure to admit to probably low
feral cat use rates, (4) failure to recognize potential cruelty to feral cats
from dirty, stiff collars, (5) failure to compensate for uncounted kills in
efficacy rate estimates, (6) failure to justify projecting results from two
seasons out over the entire year, (7) inadequate reporting of raw data, (8) failure
to justify using a product designed for bird protection even though it is
evidently unsuitable for other taxonomic groups such as herps and mammals, (9)
failure to list the tested collar models despite very different loss rates, and
(10) failure to admit that low owner acceptance means that collar covers can
mitigate only for relatively small numbers of prey items. The cat kill
reduction values reported by Willson et al. and highlighted by Birdsbesafe are therefore
unwarranted at best and deliberately misleading at worst.